Skip to main content
Log in

Sex Differences in Infants’ Visual Interest in Toys

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

An Erratum to this article was published on 14 November 2009

Abstract

Evidence indicating that sex-linked toy preferences exist in two nonhuman primate species support the hypothesis that developmental sex differences such as those observed in children’s object preferences are shaped in part by inborn factors. If so, then preferences for sex-linked toys may emerge in children before any self-awareness of gender identity and gender–congruent behavior. In order to test this hypothesis, interest in a doll and a toy truck was measured in 30 infants ranging in age from 3 to 8 months using eye-tracking technology that provides precise indicators of visual attention. Consistent with primary hypothesis, sex differences in visual interest in sex-linked toys were found, such that girls showed a visual preference (> 1.0) for the doll over the toy truck and boys compared to girls showed a greater number of visual fixations on the truck (d = .78). Our findings suggest that the conceptual categories of “masculine” and “feminine” toys are preceded by sex differences in the preferences for perceptual features associated with such objects. The existence of these innate preferences for object features coupled with well-documented social influences may explain why toy preferences are one of the earliest known manifestations of sex-linked social behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alexander, G. M. (2003). An evolutionary perspective of sex-typed toy preferences: Pink, blue, and the brain. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, 7–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, G. M., & Hines, M. (1994). Gender labels and play styles: Their relative contribution to children’s selection of playmates. Child Development, 65, 869–879.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, G. M., & Hines, M. (2002). Sex differences in responses to children’s toys in a non-human primate (cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus). Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 467–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aslin, R. N. (2007). What’s in a look? Developmental Science, 10, 48–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, S. A., & Hines, M. (1992). Early androgens are related to childhood sex-typed toy preferences. Psychological Science, 3, 203–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, J. H. (1983). Differential premises arising from differential socialization of the sexes: Some conjectures. Child Development, 54, 1335–1354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A., Shirley, L., & Caygill, L. (2002). Sex-typed preferences in three domains: Do two-year-olds need cognitive variables. British Journal of Psychology, 93, 203–217.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A., Shirley, L., & Heywood, C. (2000). Infants’ visual preference for sex-congruent babies, children, toys and activities: A longitudinal study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18, 479–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connellan, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Batki, A., & Ahluwalia, J. (2000). Sex differences in human neonatal social perception. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, 113–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connor, J. M., & Serbin, L. A. (1977). Behaviorally based masculine- and feminine-activity preferences scales for preschoolers: Correlates with other classroom behaviors and cognitive tests. Child Development, 48, 1411–1416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., Murray, E., & Hite, T. (1982). Children’s reasoning regarding sex-typed toy choices. Child Development, 53, 81–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fagot, B. I. (1977). Consequences of moderate cross-gender behavior in preschool children. Child Development, 48, 902–907.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fagot, B. I. (1978). The influence of sex of child on parental reactions to toddler children. Child Development, 49, 459–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fagot, B. I., Leinbach, M. D., & Hagan, R. (1986). Gender labeling and the adoption of sex-typed behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 22, 440–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fagot, B. I., & Littman, I. (1976). Relation of preschool sex-typing to intellectual performance in elementary school. Psychological Reports, 39, 699–704.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (1999). Male, female: The evolution of human sex differences. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, S., & Lewis, M. (1969). Play behavior in the year-old infant: Early sex differences. Child Development, 40, 21–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hassett, J. M., Siebert, E. R., & Wallen, K. (2008). Sex differences in rhesus monkey toy preferences parallel those of children. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 359–364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayhoe, M. M. (2004). Advances in relating eye movements and cognition. Infancy, 6, 267–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higley, J. D., Hopkins, W. D., Hirsch, R. M., Marra, L. M., & Suomi, S. M. (1987). Preferences of female rhesus monkeys (macaca mulatta) for infantile coloration. Developmental Psychobiology, 20, 7–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hines, M., & Alexander, G. M. (2008). Monkeys, girls, boys and toys: A confirmation [Letter to the editor]. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 359–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. H., & Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cognitive development: The case of face recognition. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., & Weinraub, M. (1979). The origins of early sex role development. Sex Roles, 5, 135–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liben, L. S., & Bigler, R. S. (2002). The developmental course of gender differentiation: Conceptualizing, measuring, and evaluating constructs and pathways. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67, 1–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52, 1119–1134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 903–933.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mather, G., & Murdoch, L. (1994). Gender discrimination in biological motion displays based on dynamic cues. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. B, 258, 273–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, C. L., Younger, B. A., & Morse, P. A. (1982). The categorization of male and female voices in infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 5, 143–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nordenstrom, A., Servin, A., Bohlin, G., Larsson, A., & Wedell, A. (2002). Sex-typed toy play behavior correlates with the degree of prenatal androgen exposure assessed by cyp21 genotype in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 87, 5119–5124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M., & Huston, A. C. (1985). Development of sex-typed play behavior in toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 21, 866–871.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pasterski, V. L., Geffner, M. E., Brain, C., Hindmarsh, P., Brook, C., & Hines, M. (2005). Prenatal hormones and postnatal socialization by parents as determinants of male-typical toy play in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Child Development, 76, 264–278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pomerleau, A., Bolduc, D., Malcuit, G., & Cossette, L. (1990). Pink or blue: Environmental gender stereotypes in the first two years of life. Sex Roles, 22, 359–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, P. D., Kelly, D. J., Lee, K., Pascalis, O., & Slater, A. M. (2008). Preference for attractive faces in human infants extends beyond conspecifics. Developmental Science, 11, 76–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robert, M., & Heroux, G. (2004). Visuo-spatial play experience: Forerunner of visuo-spatial achievement in preadolescent and adolescent boys and girls? Infant and Child Development, 13, 49–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P. (1989). Object manipulation and exploration in 2- to 5-month old infants. Developmental Psychology, 25, 871–884.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2006). Gender development. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.), and N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 858–932). New York: Wiley.

  • Serbin, L. A., & Connor, J. M. (1979). Sex-typing of children’s play preferences and patterns of cognitive performance. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 134, 315–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Serbin, L. A., Poulin-Dubois, D., Colburne, K. A., Sen, M. G., & Eichstedt, J. A. (2001). Gender stereotyping in infancy: Visual preferences for and knowledge of gender-stereotyped toys in the second year of life. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 7–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Servin, A., Bohlin, G., & Berlin, L. (1999). Sex differences in 1-, 3-, 5-year-olds’ toy-choice in a structured play-session. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 40, 43–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silk, J. B., Alberts, S. C., & Altmann, J. (2003). Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science, 302, 1231–1233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. K., & Daglish, L. (1977). Sex differences in parent and infant behavior in the home. Child Development, 48, 1250–1254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Voyer, D., Nolan, C., & Voyer, S. (2000). The relation between experience and spatial performance in men and women. Sex Roles, 43, 891–915.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Younger, B. A., & Johnson, K. E. (2006). Infants’ developing appreciation of similarities between model objects and their real-world referents. Child Development, 77, 1680–1697.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zemach, I., Chang, S., & Teller, D. Y. (2007). Infant color vision: Prediction of infants’ spontaneous color preferences. Vision Research, 47, 1368–1381.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zosuls, K. M., Ruble, D. N., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shrout, P. E., Bornstein, M. H., & Greulich, F. K. (in press). The acquisition of gender labels in infancy: Implications for sex-typed play. Developmental Psychology.

Download references

Acknowledgement

This research was supported by NSF grant BCS-0618411 (GMA).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gerianne M. Alexander.

Additional information

An erratum to this article can be found at http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-009-9581-8

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Alexander, G.M., Wilcox, T. & Woods, R. Sex Differences in Infants’ Visual Interest in Toys. Arch Sex Behav 38, 427–433 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-008-9430-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-008-9430-1

Keywords

Navigation